December 17, 2024
Intrinsic reward and word learning
From infancy onwards, humans display an innate motivation to acquire language and to communicate. We start acquiring words as babies and continue to do so throughout our lives. In fact, children are thought to learn over 3000 new words each year. However, relatively little work has focused on why we are motivated to learn words, particularly when it comes to adolescents.
Adolescence is a period of great neurological change and sensitivity to learning. It's also marked by changes in reading behaviours. Whilst children become skilled readers between the ages of 10 and 18, this period is also associated with motivational changes in reading, with teens frequently showing a disinclination to read for pleasure. This change often coincides with the transition from primary to secondary school. Policy makers are keen to motivate adolescents to read for pleasure, arguing that it would improve not just literacy outcomes, but also result in substantial economic and societal benefit.
Recent research has shown that adults find learning words to be intrinsically rewarding, and that this intrinsic reward facilitates the entrance of new words into long-term memory. Our researchers of the month, Professor Saloni Krishnan and Professor Pablo Ripollés, set out to establish whether the same is true for children and teens.
Summary
So, what is intrinsic reward?
An extrinsic reward is external to us. We could be given money every time we do a particular thing, or we might feel good about ourselves when someone tells us “well done”. Extrinsic rewards are important and help to keep our levels of motivation high. In contrast, intrinsic rewards happen without external feedback.
For the majority of time when we are learning or doing something new, we don't receive external feedback. Yet something motivates us to keep learning. Why? Well, in our brains, there is a close relationship between our reward and memory systems. Dopamine is one of the most important neurotransmitters in the brain and it is released when we find something pleasurable or rewarding. When dopamine is released, it becomes easier for whatever we are doing to enter our long-term memory. In the case of learning, when we find a learning experience to be pleasurable, our reward network is activated, releasing dopamine, and this actually enhances our ability to learn and remember.
Studies have shown that when we learn on our own, and we are aware that we’ve learned something, this experience is intrinsically rewarding. And if the experience is intrinsically rewarding, and dopamine is released, we also end up learning it better.
What did the study show?
The research team asked 345 children and young people aged 10-18 to extract word meanings from sentences. Participants were presented with sentences which contained novel words. For example, they were given a nonsense word with no known meaning, such as "Kiche". They were then provided with a couple of sentences, such as, "Old people sometimes lose their kiche" and, "Mina has beautiful long kiche." They were asked to read the sentence, then type out the meaning of the word (which here, they might have deduced to be "hair"), before rating how much they enjoyed working out what it meant, how tired they were and how confident they were in the meaning that they ascribed to it. They did not receive any explicit feedback. The young people were also asked to recall word meanings the following day. This data was analysed to see whether adolescents show the same reward patterns as adults, and whether they experience the same 'buzz' during learning. As a control, in some of the sentences, it was not possible to extract any word meaning. In these examples, the ‘correct’ answer was to note that a meaning could not be ascribed.
The results showed clear evidence of an increase in pleasure during successful word learning, and that young people's experience of reward remained fairly stable across the age range of participants. Interestingly, a sense of enjoyment was only experienced when they were reading sentences where word meaning could be worked out, not upon completion of the control questions. It seems that reward systems in the brain were stimulated by the acquisition of tangible, new knowledge, rather than by the challenge of problem solving in and of itself.
So, what makes word learning intrinsically rewarding? Firstly, the experiment allowed participants to generate and test a prediction, something that is known to create optimal conditions for learning. Like adults, children and adolescents feel pleasure when successfully engaging internal learning processes. Professor Krishnan and Professor Ripollés note that this may be due to an increase in feelings of self-efficacy and confidence, and a reduction in feelings of uncertainty, when the young people felt that they had learned something. For learning experiences to be intrinsically rewarding, we need to feel sufficiently confident that we can learn or have learned.
Implications
"Word learning is pleasurable for children and adults. This is an important finding as it will ultimately allow us to develop strategies to drive engagement with language and reading, optimising learning experiences in childhood."
From a research perspective, we don’t yet know much about how to target intrinsic reward when it comes to language learning and so our researchers of the month will be investigating the practical implications of this study in the future. However, they do have some top tips which might help to boost children's sense of intrinsic reward when it comes to reading and word learning.
When reading with children, take time to stop and consider word meanings. Talking to children about what they think new words mean and helping develop their confidence in ascribing meanings to words can broaden their vocabulary and promote word learning.
Give children choice and a sense of agency. Providing variety and options when it comes to books, reading interventions and programmes is known to boost young people's sense of enjoyment and motivation.
Capitalise on their interests. Talking to children and young people about subjects that interest them can help to build new vocabulary, encourage them to make connections between words and may nudge them to build certainty and confidence around language within this specific domain. Foster things that they find rewarding. As a general rule, we tend to be very good at doing this with younger children, but frequently less so as they get older. Keep going!
Model an enjoyment of reading and word learning. Signal when you learn a new words. Be explicit about it!
Talk about the nuances of words. Building a wide ranging vocabulary around emotions and experiences can help to cultivate empathy and a greater capacity to navigate social and emotional challenges.
Professor Saloni Krishnan and Professor Pablo Ripollés
Professor Krishnan is Associate Professor in Developmental Language Sciences at University College London and Professor Ripollés is Assistant Professor of Psychology at New York University
Professor Saloni Krishnan is a developmental cognitive neuroscientist and her research focuses on understanding childhood communication disorders, such as DLD (developmental language disorder) and dyslexia. She has been awarded funding from the Academy of Medical Sciences, the MRC and the ESRC to support my research. With her MRC New Investigator grant, she is leading a team focusing on investigating the links between motivation and language learning in neurodivergent children. She has won several prizes for my research, including the Neil O’Connor Award from the British Psychological Society and the NDAS Mid-Career Prize. She was named a Rising Star by the Association of Psychological Sciences in 2022. Her research has featured on the BBC, the Guardian, Daily Mail, and the Boston Globe.
Professor Pablo Ripollés is an Assistant Professor with a joint position between the Department of Psychology and the Music and Audio Research Laboratory (MARL) at New York University. He has served as MARL's associate director since September 2020. He received a B.M. in Computer Engineering from University of València (2009), an MSc in Biomedical Engineering from the University of Navarra (2011), and a PhD in Biomedicine from the University of Barcelona (2016). His research focuses on building bridges between different fields of cognitive neurosciences - especially reward - and other areas of study, including music, technology, and language. His work also goes beyond basic research and includes applied scientific studies that capitalise on music to improve human health and wellbeing.
Professor Saloni Krishnan and Professor Pablo Ripollés
Professor Krishnan is Associate Professor in Developmental Language Sciences at University College London and Professor Ripollés is Assistant Professor of Psychology at New York University
Scroll our research gallery
Nov 14, 2024
Delivering effective menstrual education
Research has shown that menstruation and menstrual cycle symptoms can be disruptive and detrimental to physical, mental and social wellbeing. As many as 93% of teen girls experience dysmenorrhea (severe cramps) and girls with a typical cycle experience 11 menstrual cycle-related symptoms on average, potentially including mood changes, stomach cramps and increased levels of anxiety and fatigue. Adolescents with problematic symptoms have reported a negative impact on school attendance, behaviour, confidence around peers and participation in physical activity. In the United Kingdom, evidence suggests that menstrual health education delivered by teachers generally focuses on menstrual cycle biology, as opposed to management of menstruation and menstrual cycle related symptoms. Our latest researcher of the month, Dr Natalie Brown, set out to understand young people’s perceptions of menstrual education in schools and their experiences of menstruating whilst at school, including within physical education. Her study involved nine focus groups with 10-15 year old female pupils attending primary and secondary schools across England and Wales. Pupils highlighted that, in their experience, menstrual education is limited and noted that the menstrual cycle impacts negatively on their school experience, affecting participation in sport and school attendance generally. Participants highlighted a need for more support and information regarding understanding and managing periods in school.
Oct 17, 2024
How influential is social media on young people's mental health?
A significant body of evidence tells us that young people's mental health has decreased over time, and a potential link between social media use and this mental health decline has triggered wide interest and concern. Research into social media use and mental health is being conducted at a rapid pace but, so far, findings are far from conclusive. Our new Researcher of the Month, Dr Margarita Panayiotou, adds to this body of work. She led a team of researchers who recently published a study involving thousands of young people in the UK. It set out to explore the interplay between social media use and teens' mental health, wellbeing and social life, and found that when considering the complex combination of multiple factors, things such as a lack of family support are arguably more important for adolescent mental health than social media use.
Sep 16, 2024
How do young children’s relationships with parents and teachers impact their adjustment to school?
We already know that children's relationships with their parents and teachers contribute to their school adjustment and achievement, and that positive, close relationships with adults are really important for a wide variety of children’s outcomes. However, few studies have examined interactions between these relationships or sought to address the nuances of children’s experiences in terms of how different relationships have different impact or influence. To address this, along with colleagues, our new Researcher of the Month, Dr Caoimhe Dempsey, has recently published an article which aims to examine the links between children's relationships with their mothers, fathers and teachers, and three domains of school adjustment: academic achievement, academic self-concept and behavioural adjustment in the classroom. By examining a range of data, the researchers sought to find out whether some children are more susceptible to the effects of the quality of their relationship with their teacher.
Aug 19, 2024
Supporting children's transition to secondary school
Supporting children to successfully transition from primary to secondary or senior school is vitally important. It's a big life change, which can leave both parents and children feeling excited and nervous in equal measure. At Tooled Up, we often use the analogy of a journey for the transitional experience. In fact, anyone who has attended or watched a talk with our founder, Dr Kathy Weston, is likely to have heard her say that we'd all want our children to be well prepared for any journey they go on, and that, as loving parents and carers, we wouldn't dream of sending them off on any journey without the right equipment, mindset and strategies to reach their destination safely and securely. Transition to a new school is no different. Research shows that there are numerous holistic protective factors that can help to ease transition. Conversely, it's also the case that problems with successfully transitioning to secondary school and subsequent lower levels of school connectedness are associated with lower education outcomes, school drop-out, higher levels of depression and anxiety, and increased involvement in criminal, violent and antisocial behaviour. Research also shows that these difficulties may not be equal across different socio-demographic variables, with children from underserved communities (including those from minority ethnic backgrounds and lower socio-economic status) and children with behavioural difficulties facing greater challenges during the transition to secondary school. It's therefore important for all parents and educators to consider how to make this transition as seamless as possible. Along with her co-authors, our Researcher of the Month, Dr Aurelie Lange, has published a new paper which evaluates the efficacy of a new UK-based online intervention called Level Up. In it, Dr Lange seeks to explore families' experiences of facilitators and barriers to engagement and change.
Jul 15, 2024
The impact of digital experiences on teens with mental health vulnerabilities
Over the last decade, digital developments have led to major changes in the ways that teens learn, work, play and interact with others. Digital access is a daily reality for most children and young people. Nine in 10 children in the UK now own a smartphone by the time they reach the age of 11, and the large majority of children aged 11 now use social media (78%) and have a social media profile (72%), despite being younger than the minimum age requirement for many platforms. By the time teens are 17 years old, 97% will use social media. Coincident with this change, adolescent mental health problems have also increased in prevalence. It is therefore no surprise that much research has focused on the relationship between digital experiences and adolescent mental health. However, comparatively few studies have directly compared the experiences of teens with mental health conditions meaning that some key questions remain unanswered. Do adolescents with pre‐existing mental health conditions differ in terms of why and how they engage with the digital world compared to peers without such difficulties? Are specific mental health conditions linked to different patterns of digital usage? What role do such differences play in the development and escalation of these conditions? In a recent paper, our Researcher of the Month, Dr Kasia Kostyrka-Allchorne, and her colleagues, explore what the evidence shows about the digital experiences of teens with mental health vulnerabilities.
Jun 13, 2024
NELI Preschool: a new oral language enrichment programme for preschools and nurseries
Oral language skills provide the foundation for formal education, yet many children enter school with language weaknesses. Oral language is fundamental to children’s overall development and educational success. It is linked to all higher level cognitive skills and is pivotal for literacy development, and education more generally. It’s also vitally important for children’s social and emotional development. The term oral language refers to a complex set of skills that should ideally work seamlessly together to enable children to communicate with others by producing and understanding language. It’s an umbrella term, encompassing numerous component skills which include having a good vocabulary and the grammatical ability to combine words effectively to convey meaning, along with an understanding of cause and effect, memory skills and the ability to plan what to say and what not to say. Language skills develop rapidly between the ages of 3–6 years making preschool an excellent time to intervene to support language development. Because of this, Dr Gill West and her colleagues – as part of a team headed by Professor Charles Hulme – have developed and evaluated the efficacy of a new language enrichment programme, the Nuffield Early Language Intervention—Preschool (NELI Preschool), which is delivered to children the year before they enter formal education. The programme combines language enrichment for all children, with additional targeted support for those with language needs, potentially narrowing the gap in language skills associated with social disadvantage.
May 16, 2024
"More is more”: the impact of careers education on later outcomes
Does school-based careers education, advice, information and guidance (CEAIG) have any influence on later life outcomes? A body of research has shown that careers education and activities can play an important role in helping young people to prepare for and navigate transitions into the world of work. It has also shown that the quality and quantity of provision can vary considerably, with those who are most socially disadvantaged experiencing the least and lowest quality provision. However, until now, the impact of careers education on later life outcomes has remained unclear. A new paper written by Dr Julie Moote and colleagues seeks to find out what relationship there is between young people’s perceptions of the quality and quantity of school-based careers activities and education experienced at the age of 14–16 and their outcomes several years later, when they are 21–22. Findings are discussed in the light of recent legislation relating to careers support in England, which highlights how important it is to ensure that the needs of all young people transitioning into the workforce and adulthood are met.
Apr 11, 2024
STEM in preschool settings: do teachers’ scientific questions differ by child gender?
In the US, as in numerous countries including the UK, female and non-White students are underrepresented in science, technology, engineering, and maths (STEM) fields. A significant body of research demonstrates significant gender disparities in STEM achievement, participation and attitudes from early childhood, and that these continue to widen as children get older. Children are strongly influenced by their early environments and conversations with adults. Whilst gendered messages around STEM are now rarely explicit – children are unlikely to be told that girls can’t be scientists – they may receive more subtle messages about representation and belonging. These differences may contribute to children’s beliefs about their own STEM ability, their performance in STEM activities, and their decision to pursue a career in STEM. US researcher, Dr Sona Kumar, and her colleagues sought to explore whether there are subtle differences in the messages that preschool educators send girls and boys about STEM. Previous, older research with caregivers found that parents tended to provide more scientific explanations to boys than to girls. Studies have also found that, within classroom settings, boys often receive more attention from teachers on science-related subjects than girls. Dr Kumar predicted that teachers would direct more scientific questions to boys than girls and tested this by observing a preschool setting.
Mar 14, 2024
Do parental controls work and what are the pros and cons?
Policymakers and technology companies promote the use of parental control tools to ensure children’s safety in our digital age. In recent years, there has been considerable industry investment in this new generation of technical child protection measures, designed to help parents and carers to control what their children see and do online. Some of these tools operate at the level of the device (such as a phone or tablet). Other tools are embedded into specific services, such as Snapchat, or streaming services such as Netflix. Others work at the level of the operating system, such as IOS, Android or Windows, or are provided by your broadband company. Control tools vary in functionality. Some allow parents to limit the amount of time that children spend on a device or particular app. Others can track their child’s location. Internet filters block certain categories of content (violence, adult content or gambling, for example), or specific websites. There are other tools that track children’s online activity (search history, online purchases or activity logs, for example) and provide reports or real-time alerts to parents. Some tools track activity and interactions on social media platforms and can limit children’s contact with unknown users. But how many families actually make use of them, and do they work? Our Researcher of the Month, Dr Mariya Stoilova, and her colleagues, reviewed all available evidence in a recently published study.
Feb 09, 2024
BMI, body dissatisfaction and depression: positive steps forward
We know that more adolescents are experiencing depressive symptoms than in the past, but we don’t yet know precisely why. Noting a need to understand modifiable risk factors for depression, which could inform future preventative work, our researcher of the month, Dr Francesca Solmi, and her team, have recently published new and novel findings about the impact of high BMI and body dissatisfaction on children’s later mental health. They “found strong longitudinal evidence that a high BMI in childhood is linked with an increased risk of depressive symptoms multiple years later” and discovered that body dissatisfaction accounts for a significant proportion of this link, finding strong evidence that being unhappy with one’s appearance at the age of 11 is linked with increased depressive symptoms during the teen years.